-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 0
/
brainscopycut.bib
3915 lines (3611 loc) · 416 KB
/
brainscopycut.bib
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
940
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
950
951
952
953
954
955
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
976
977
978
979
980
981
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
%% This BibTeX bibliography file was created using BibDesk.
%% http://bibdesk.sourceforge.net/
%% Created for Camille Roth at 2016-07-30 04:59:44 +0200
%% Saved with string encoding Unicode (UTF-8)
@incollection{dawkins_selfish_1976,
Author = {Richard Dawkins},
Title = {Memes, The New Replicator},
Date-Added = {2016-07-30 02:59:01 +0000},
Date-Modified = {2016-07-30 02:59:35 +0000},
Pages = {189--201},
Publisher = {Oxford: Oxford University Press},
Booktitle = {The Selfish Gene},
Year = 1976}
@techreport{burri_re-conceptualizing_2011,
Abstract = {The article seeks a re-conceptualization of the global digital divide debate. It critically explores the predominant notion, its evolution and measurement, as well as the policies that have been advanced to bridge the digital divide. Acknowledging the complexity of this inequality, the article aims at analyzing the disparities beyond the connectivity and skills barriers. Without understating the first two digital divides, it is argued that as the Internet becomes more sophisticated and more integrated into economic, social, and cultural processes, a ``third'' generation of divides becomes critical. These divides are drawn not at the entry to the net but within the net itself, and limit access to content. The increasing barriers to content, though of a diverse nature, all relate to some governance characteristics inherent in cyberspace, such as global spillover of local decisions, regulation through code, and proliferation of self- and co-regulatory models. It is maintained that as the practice of intervention intensifies in cyberspace, multiple and far-reaching points of control outside formal legal institutions are created, threatening the availability of public goods and making the pursuit of public objectives difficult. This is an aspect that is rarely addressed in the global digital divide discussions, even in comprehensive analyses and political initiatives such as the World Summit on the Information Society. Yet, the conceptualization of the digital divide as impeded access to content may be key in terms of ensuring real participation and catering for the long-term implications of digital technologies.},
Address = {Rochester, NY},
Author = {Burri, Mira},
File = {Burri (2011), Re-conceptualizing the Global Digital Divide.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/X9AN8KSX/Burri (2011), Re-conceptualizing the Global Digital Divide.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/3U4QJWXK/papers.html:text/html},
Institution = {Social Science Research Network},
Keywords = {access to content, cyberlaw, global digital divide},
Month = dec,
Number = {ID 1968956},
Title = {Re-{Conceptualizing} the {Global} {Digital} {Divide}},
Type = {{SSRN} {Scholarly} {Paper}},
Year = {2011}}
@techreport{page_pagerank_1999,
Author = {Page, Lawrence and Brin, Sergey and Motwani, Rajeev and Winograd, Terry},
File = {Page et al_1999_The PageRank Citation Ranking.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/3NFUIKC9/Page et al_1999_The PageRank Citation Ranking.pdf:application/pdf},
Institution = {Stanford InfoLab},
Keywords = {google, read-ok, web},
Title = {The {PageRank} {Citation} {Ranking}: {Bringing} {Order} to the {Web}},
Year = {1999}}
@book{kroeber_nature_1952,
Address = {Chicago},
Author = {Kroeber, A. L},
Language = {English},
Note = {OCLC: 487751},
Publisher = {University of Chicago Press},
Title = {The nature of culture.},
Year = {1952}}
@article{yonelinas_nature_2002,
Abstract = {To account for dissociations observed in recognition memory tests, several dual-process models have been proposed that assume that recognition judgments can be based on the recollection of details about previous events or on the assessment of stimulus familiarity. In the current article, these models are examined, along with the methods that have been developed to measure recollection and familiarity. The relevant empirical literature from behavioral, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging studies is then reviewed in order to assess model predictions. Results from a variety of measurement methods, including task-dissociation and process-estimation methods, are found to lead to remarkably consistent conclusions about the nature of recollection and familiarity, particularly when ceiling effects are avoided. For example, recollection is found to be more sensitive than familiarity to response speeding, division of attention, generation, semantic encoding, the effects of aging, and the amnestic effects of benzodiazepines, but it is less sensitive than familiarity to shifts in response criterion, fluency manipulations, forgetting over short retention intervals, and some perceptual manipulations. Moreover, neuropsychological and neuroimaging results indicate that the two processes rely on partially distinct neural substrates and provide support for models that assume that recollection relies on the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, whereas familiarity relies on regions surrounding the hippocampus. Double dissociations produced by experimental manipulations at time of test indicate that the two processes are independent at retrieval, and single dissociations produced by study manipulations indicate that they are partially independent during encoding. Recollection is similar but not identical to free recall, whereas familiarity is similar to conceptual implicit memory, but is dissociable from perceptual implicit memory. Finally, the results indicate that recollection reflects a thresholdlike retrieval process that supports novel learning, whereas familiarity reflects a signal-detection process that can support novel learning only under certain conditions. The results verify a number of model predictions and prove useful in resolving several theoretical disagreements.},
Author = {Yonelinas, Andrew P},
Doi = {10.1006/jmla.2002.2864},
File = {ScienceDirect Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/ZS3F6GH2/S0749596X02928640.html:text/html;Yonelinas_2002_The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/SAMZRFAH/Yonelinas_2002_The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {0749-596X},
Journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
Keywords = {linguistics, memory, psycholinguistics, word familiarty, word frequency, word recall},
Month = apr,
Number = {3},
Pages = {441--517},
Shorttitle = {The {Nature} of {Recollection} and {Familiarity}},
Title = {The {Nature} of {Recollection} and {Familiarity}: {A} {Review} of 30 {Years} of {Research}},
Volume = {46},
Year = {2002},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2002.2864}}
@misc{ingold_human_2014,
Abstract = {Ingold, Tim (Huxley Memorial Lecture, 7 November 2014). On Human Correspondence. Podcast retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/royalanthro.},
Collaborator = {Ingold, Tim},
Keywords = {blob, correspondence, knot, line, longing, read-ok, undergoing},
Month = nov,
Shorttitle = {Tim {Ingold}},
Title = {On {Human} {Correspondence} -- {Huxley} {Memorial} {Lecture} 2014},
Year = {2014}}
@article{brooks_elephants_1990,
Abstract = {There is an alternative route to Artificial Intelligence that diverges from the directions pursued under that banner for the last thirty some years. The traditional approach has emphasized the abstract manipulation of symbols, whose grounding in physical reality has rarely been achieved. We explore a research methodology which emphasizes ongoing physical interaction with the environment as the primary source of constraint on the design of intelligent systems. We show how this methodology has recently had significant successes on a par with the most successful classical efforts. We outline plausible future work along these lines which can lead to vastly more ambitious systems.},
Author = {Brooks, Rodney A.},
Doi = {10.1016/S0921-8890(05)80025-9},
File = {Brooks_1990_Elephants don't play chess.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/I24E2ZVR/Brooks_1990_Elephants don't play chess.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/JAFUFDE6/S0921889005800259.html:text/html},
Issn = {0921-8890},
Journal = {Robotics and Autonomous Systems},
Keywords = {Artificial Intelligence, cybernetics, Mobile robots, Planning, read-ok, Situated activity, Subsumption architecture},
Month = jun,
Number = {1--2},
Pages = {3--15},
Series = {Designing {Autonomous} {Agents}},
Title = {Elephants don't play chess},
Volume = {6},
Year = {1990},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0921-8890(05)80025-9}}
@article{marian_clearpond:_2012,
Abstract = {Past research has demonstrated cross-linguistic, cross-modal, and task-dependent differences in neighborhood density effects, indicating a need to control for neighborhood variables when developing and interpreting research on language processing. The goals of the present paper are two-fold: (1) to introduce CLEARPOND (Cross-Linguistic Easy-Access Resource for Phonological and Orthographic Neighborhood Densities), a centralized database of phonological and orthographic neighborhood information, both within and between languages, for five commonly-studied languages: Dutch, English, French, German, and Spanish; and (2) to show how CLEARPOND can be used to compare general properties of phonological and orthographic neighborhoods across languages. CLEARPOND allows researchers to input a word or list of words and obtain phonological and orthographic neighbors, neighborhood densities, mean neighborhood frequencies, word lengths by number of phonemes and graphemes, and spoken-word frequencies. Neighbors can be defined by substitution, deletion, and/or addition, and the database can be queried separately along each metric or summed across all three. Neighborhood values can be obtained both within and across languages, and outputs can optionally be restricted to neighbors of higher frequency. To enable researchers to more quickly and easily develop stimuli, CLEARPOND can also be searched by features, generating lists of words that meet precise criteria, such as a specific range of neighborhood sizes, lexical frequencies, and/or word lengths. CLEARPOND is freely-available to researchers and the public as a searchable, online database and for download at http://clearpond.northwestern.edu .},
Author = {Marian, Viorica and Bartolotti, James and Chabal, Sarah and Shook, Anthony},
Doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0043230},
File = {Marian et al_2012_CLEARPOND.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/K4DXKQ7B/Marian et al_2012_CLEARPOND.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/5H434ZXB/article.html:text/html},
Issn = {1932-6203},
Journal = {PLOS ONE},
Keywords = {Database searching, language, Multilingualism, Phonemes, Phonology, psycholinguistics, vocabulary, Web-based applications},
Month = aug,
Number = {8},
Pages = {e43230},
Shorttitle = {{CLEARPOND}},
Title = {{CLEARPOND}: {Cross}-{Linguistic} {Easy}-{Access} {Resource} for {Phonological} and {Orthographic} {Neighborhood} {Densities}},
Volume = {7},
Year = {2012},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043230}}
@misc{_construction_2015,
Abstract = {In linguistics, construction grammar groups a number of models of grammar that all subscribe to the idea that knowledge of a language is based on a collection of "form and function pairings". The "function" side covers what is commonly understood as meaning, content, or intent; it usually extends over both conventional fields of semantics and pragmatics.
Such pairs are learnt by hearing them being used frequently enough by others. Uses of constructions may happen and be acquired in mainstream or everyday language, but also in linguistic subcultures that are using a sociolect, dialect, or in formal contexts using standard languages or jargon associated with greater sociolinguistic prestige in comparison to plain language.
Construction grammar (often abridged CxG) is thus a kind of metalinguistic model, letting the door open to a variety of linguistic theories. It is typically associated with cognitive linguistics, partly because many of the linguists that are involved in construction grammar are also involved in cognitive linguistics, and partly because construction grammar and cognitive linguistics share many theoretical and philosophical foundations.},
Copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/5TQPQWAR/index.html:text/html},
Journal = {Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia},
Keywords = {read-ok},
Language = {en},
Month = oct,
Note = {Page Version ID: 687227560},
Title = {Construction grammar},
Year = {2015}}
@misc{_symbol_2016,
Abstract = {The symbol grounding problem is related to the problem of how words (symbols) get their meanings, and hence to the problem of what meaning itself really is. The problem of meaning is in turn related to the problem of consciousness, or how it is that mental states are meaningful. According to a widely held theory of cognition called "computationalism," cognition (i.e., thinking) is just a form of computation. But computation in turn is just formal symbol manipulation: symbols are manipulated according to rules that are based on the symbols' shapes, not their meanings. How are those symbols (e.g., the words in our heads) connected to the things they refer to? It cannot be through the mediation of an external interpreter's head, because that would lead to an infinite regress, just as looking up the meanings of words in a (unilingual) dictionary of a language that one does not understand would lead to an infinite regress. The symbols in an autonomous hybrid symbolic+sensorimotor system---a Turing-scale robot consisting of both a symbol system and a sensorimotor system that reliably connects its internal symbols to the external objects they refer to, so it can interact with them Turing-indistinguishably from the way a person does---would be grounded. But whether its symbols would have meaning rather than just grounding is something that even the robotic Turing test---hence cognitive science itself---cannot determine, or explain.},
Copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/A4GGISB9/index.html:text/html},
Journal = {Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia},
Keywords = {Artificial Intelligence, consciousness, read-ok, symbol system, turing test},
Language = {en},
Month = jan,
Note = {Page Version ID: 698784873},
Title = {Symbol grounding problem},
Year = {2016}}
@incollection{vasudevi_reddy_puzzle_2008,
Author = {{Vasudevi Reddy}},
Booktitle = {How {Infants} {Know} {Minds}},
File = {Vasudevi Reddy_2008_A Puzzle - Minding the Gap.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/HBTBQDFJ/Vasudevi Reddy_2008_A Puzzle - Minding the Gap.pdf:application/pdf},
Title = {A {Puzzle} - {Minding} the {Gap}},
Year = {2008}}
@article{millman_python_2011,
Abstract = {Python has arguably become the de facto standard for exploratory, interactive, and computation-driven scientific research. This issue discusses Python\'s advantages for scientific research and presents several of the core Python libraries and tools used in scientific research.},
Author = {Millman, K. Jarrod and Aivazis, Michael},
Doi = {10.1109/MCSE.2011.36},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/M8FHAN4B/MCSE.2011.html:text/html},
Issn = {1521-9615},
Journal = {Computing in Science \& Engineering},
Keywords = {Engineers},
Month = mar,
Number = {2},
Pages = {9--12},
Title = {Python for {Scientists} and {Engineers}},
Volume = {13},
Year = {2011},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/MCSE.2011.36}}
@article{lauf_analyzing_2013,
Author = {Lauf, Aurelien and Valette, Mathieu and Khouas, Leila},
File = {Lauf et al_2013_Analyzing Variation Patterns In Quotes Over Time.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/Z4K9J5N4/Lauf et al_2013_Analyzing Variation Patterns In Quotes Over Time.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/M56F593X/Analyzing Variation Patterns In Quotes Over Time.html:text/html},
Issn = {1870-4069},
Journal = {Research in Computing Science},
Keywords = {co-reference, discourse analysis, linguistics, memes, modality, multiple sequence alignment, quotes, read-abstract},
Language = {en},
Pages = {223--232},
Title = {Analyzing {Variation} {Patterns} {In} {Quotes} {Over} {Time}},
Volume = {70},
Year = {2013}}
@book{wheeler_evolution_2002,
Abstract = {Ever since Darwin, scholars have noted that cultural entities such as languages, laws, firms and theories seem to 'evolve' through sequences of variation, selection and replication, in many ways just like living organisms. These essays consider whether this comparison is 'just a metaphor', or whether modern evolutionary theory can help us to understand the dynamics of different cultural domains. The 'evolutionary paradigm of rationality' has a significant role to play throughout the human sciences, but raises complex issues in every cultural context where it is applied. By fostering discussion between scholars from a wide range of research traditions, this volume aims to influence the evolution of all of them.},
Author = {Wheeler, Michael and Ziman, John and Boden, Margaret A.},
Isbn = {978-0-19-726262-7},
Keywords = {Biological Evolution, Cultural evolution, developmental system, Social Science / Anthropology / Cultural},
Language = {en},
Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
Title = {The {Evolution} of {Cultural} {Entities}},
Year = {2002}}
@article{kashima_recovering_2000,
Abstract = {Despite the current treatment of Frederic C. Bartlett as a cognitive psychologist, his psychology was fundamentally socio-cultural. More than half a century after the publication of Psychology and Primitive Culture (1923) and Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology (1932), his theoretical contribution has contemporary implications for a social psychology that takes culture seriously. This paper seeks to recover and appraise Bartlett's social psychology in light of the recent literature on culture and psychology, and discusses his relevance for a social psychology of cultural dynamics. Copyright {\copyright} 2000 John Wiley \& Sons, Ltd.},
Author = {Kashima, Yoshihisa},
Copyright = {Copyright {\copyright} 2000 John Wiley \& Sons, Ltd.},
Doi = {10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(200005/06)30:3<383::AID-EJSP996>3.0.CO;2-C},
File = {Kashima_2000_Recovering Bartlett's social psychology of cultural dynamics.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/28X8Z525/Kashima_2000_Recovering Bartlett's social psychology of cultural dynamics.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/2KE4FSDI/abstract.html:text/html},
Issn = {1099-0992},
Journal = {European Journal of Social Psychology},
Keywords = {Bartlett, cultural dynamics, read-abstract, read-part, social psychology, transmission chains},
Language = {en},
Month = may,
Number = {3},
Pages = {383--403},
Title = {Recovering {Bartlett}'s social psychology of cultural dynamics},
Volume = {30},
Year = {2000},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(200005/06)30:3%3C383::AID-EJSP996%3E3.0.CO;2-C}}
@article{yarkoni_cognitive_2010,
Abstract = {Cognitive neuroscientists increasingly recognize that continued progress in understanding human brain function will require not only the acquisition of new data, but also the synthesis and integration of data across studies and laboratories. Here we review ongoing efforts to develop a more cumulative science of human brain function. We discuss the rationale for an increased focus on formal synthesis of the cognitive neuroscience literature, provide an overview of recently developed tools and platforms designed to facilitate the sharing and integration of neuroimaging data, and conclude with a discussion of several emerging developments that hold even greater promise in advancing the study of human brain function.},
Author = {Yarkoni, Tal and Poldrack, Russell A. and Van Essen, David C. and Wager, Tor D.},
Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.004},
File = {Yarkoni et al_2010_Cognitive neuroscience 2.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/ZRVW5G3U/Yarkoni et al_2010_Cognitive neuroscience 2.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {1364-6613},
Journal = {Trends in cognitive sciences},
Keywords = {cognitive atlas, meta-analyses, neuroscience, read-next},
Month = nov,
Number = {11},
Pages = {489--496},
Shorttitle = {Cognitive neuroscience 2.0},
Title = {Cognitive neuroscience 2.0: building a cumulative science of human brain function},
Volume = {14},
Year = {2010},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.004}}
@incollection{weaver_science_1991,
Abstract = {Science has led to a multitude of results that affect men's lives. Some of these results are embodied in mere conveniences of a relatively trivial sort. Many of them, based on science and developed through technology, are essential to the machinery of modern life. Many other results, especially those associated with the biological and medical sciences, are of unquestioned benefit and comfort. Certain aspects of science have profoundly influenced men's ideas and even their ideals. Still other aspects of science are thoroughly awesome.},
Author = {Weaver, Warren},
Booktitle = {Facets of {Systems} {Science}},
Copyright = {{\copyright}1991 Springer-Verlag US},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/VEHZD8W2/978-1-4899-0718-9_30.html:text/html;Weaver_1991_Science and Complexity.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/KJ2PRF9D/Weaver_1991_Science and Complexity.pdf:application/pdf},
Isbn = {978-1-4899-0720-2 978-1-4899-0718-9},
Keywords = {Complexity, complex systems, read-ok, science, Social Sciences, general},
Language = {en},
Note = {DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4899-0718-9\_30},
Number = {7},
Pages = {449--456},
Publisher = {Springer US},
Series = {International {Federation} for {Systems} {Research} {International} {Series} on {Systems} {Science} and {Engineering}},
Title = {Science and {Complexity}},
Year = {1991}}
@misc{cosmides_evolutionary_1997,
Author = {Cosmides, Leda and Tooby, John},
File = {Evolutionary Psychology Primer by Leda Cosmides and John Tooby:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/DXQ7NEBK/primer.html:text/html},
Keywords = {adaptation, emotion, evolutionary biology, evolutionary psychology, function, instinct, nature nurture, read-ok, social contract, social exchange, social reasoning, wason selection task},
Title = {Evolutionary {Psychology}: {A} {Primer}},
Year = {1997}}
@misc{_language-game_2016,
Abstract = {A language-game (German: Sprachspiel) is a philosophical concept developed by Ludwig Wittgenstein and Friedrich Waismann, referring to simple examples of language use and the actions into which the language is woven.},
Copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/2P3T5MGM/index.html:text/html},
Journal = {Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia},
Keywords = {read-ok, wittgenstein},
Language = {en},
Month = feb,
Note = {Page Version ID: 703234789},
Title = {Language-game (philosophy)},
Year = {2016}}
@article{nelson_how_2013,
Abstract = {Free-association norms indicate that words are organized into semantic/associative neighborhoods within a larger network of words and links that bind the net together. We present evidence indicating that memory for a recent word event can depend on implicitly and simultaneously activating related words in its neighborhood. Processing a word during encoding primes its network representation as a function of the density of the links in its neighborhood. Such priming increases recall and recognition and can have long-lasting effects when the word is processed in working memory. Evidence for this phenomenon is reviewed in extralist-cuing, primed free-association, intralist-cuing, and single-item recognition tasks. The findings also show that when a related word is presented in order to cue the recall of a studied word, the cue activates the target in an array of related words that distract and reduce the probability of the target's selection. The activation of the semantic network produces priming benefits during encoding, and search costs during retrieval. In extralist cuing, recall is a negative function of cue-to-distractor strength, and a positive function of neighborhood density, cue-to-target strength, and target-to-cue strength. We show how these four measures derived from the network can be combined and used to predict memory performance. These measures play different roles in different tasks, indicating that the contribution of the semantic network varies with the context provided by the task. Finally, we evaluate spreading-activation and quantum-like entanglement explanations for the priming effects produced by neighborhood density.},
Author = {Nelson, Douglas L. and Kitto, Kirsty and Galea, David and McEvoy, Cathy L. and Bruza, Peter D.},
Doi = {10.3758/s13421-013-0312-y},
File = {Nelson et al_2013_How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/MSGZ4QW9/Nelson et al_2013_How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/D4AQ5UX4/s13421-013-0312-y.html:text/html},
Issn = {0090-502X, 1532-5946},
Journal = {Memory \& Cognition},
Keywords = {clustering coefficient, free association, priming, read-abstract, semantic network, word recognition, working memory},
Language = {en},
Month = may,
Number = {6},
Pages = {797--819},
Title = {How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to event memory},
Volume = {41},
Year = {2013},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0312-y}}
@article{de_jaegher_can_2010,
Abstract = {An important shift is taking place in social cognition research, away from a focus on the individual mind and toward embodied and participatory aspects of social understanding. Empirical results already imply that social cognition is not reducible to the workings of individual cognitive mechanisms. To galvanize this interactive turn, we provide an operational definition of social interaction and distinguish the different explanatory roles -- contextual, enabling and constitutive -- it can play in social cognition. We show that interactive processes are more than a context for social cognition: they can complement and even replace individual mechanisms. This new explanatory power of social interaction can push the field forward by expanding the possibilities of scientific explanation beyond the individual.},
Author = {De Jaegher, Hanne and Di Paolo, Ezequiel and Gallagher, Shaun},
Doi = {10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009},
File = {De Jaegher et al_2010_Can social interaction constitute social cognition.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/ZG88NMGZ/De Jaegher et al_2010_Can social interaction constitute social cognition.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/WRA35HK2/S1364-6613(10)00146-4.html:text/html},
Issn = {1364-6613},
Journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
Keywords = {read-now, social cognition, social interaction},
Language = {English},
Month = jan,
Number = {10},
Pages = {441--447},
Title = {Can social interaction constitute social cognition?},
Volume = {14},
Year = {2010},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009}}
@article{mesoudi_hierarchical_2004,
Abstract = {There is extensive evidence that adults, children, and some non-human species, represent routine events in the form of hierarchically structured \'action scripts,\' and show superior recall and imitation of information at relatively high-levels of this hierarchy. Here we investigate the hypothesis that a \'hierarchical bias\' operates in human cultural transmission, acting to impose a hierarchical structure onto descriptions of everyday events, and to increasingly describe those events in terms of higher hierarchical levels. Descriptions of three everyday events (going to a restaurant, getting up and going shopping) expressed entirely in terms of basic low-level actions were transmitted along ten chains each containing four adult human participants. It was found that the proportion of low-level information showed a significant linear decrease with transmission generation, while the proportions of medium- and high-level information showed significant linear increases, consistent with the operation of a hierarchical bias. The findings additionally provide support for script theory in general, and are discussed in relation to hierarchical imitation in non-human primates.},
Author = {Mesoudi, Alex and Whiten, Andrew},
Doi = {10.1163/156853704323074732},
File = {Mesoudi_Whiten_2004_The Hierarchical Transformation of Event Knowledge in Human Cultural.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/NUPJVDEQ/Mesoudi_Whiten_2004_The Hierarchical Transformation of Event Knowledge in Human Cultural.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/CFBKB4HJ/156853704323074732.html:text/html},
Issn = {1568-5373},
Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Culture},
Keywords = {CULTURAL TRANSMISSION, EVENT KNOWLEDGE, HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE, read-abstract, subjective organization, transmission chains},
Month = mar,
Number = {1},
Pages = {1--24},
Title = {The {Hierarchical} {Transformation} of {Event} {Knowledge} in {Human} {Cultural} {Transmission}},
Volume = {4},
Year = {2004},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853704323074732}}
@article{stewart_why_1994,
Abstract = {The role of science is to seek simplicity in a complex world. Nature exhibits many remarkable regularities and patterns, and science works by assuming that these patterns arise from the regularities and patterns of the underlying `laws of nature' that govern them. This is a comfortable picture, which encourages a view of the relation between laws and their consequences---between cause and effect, ie simple rules imply simple behaviour, therefore complicated behaviour must arise from complicated rules. This article questions whether chaos and complexity theories hold the answer to the mysteries of emergent phenomena such as mind, consciousness, biological form and social structure.},
Author = {Stewart, Ian and Cohen, Jack},
Doi = {10.1016/0016-3287(94)90035-3},
File = {ScienceDirect Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/6WTS6XQG/0016328794900353.html:text/html;Stewart_Cohen_1994_Why are there simple rules in a complicated universe.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/24TJ7SKJ/Stewart_Cohen_1994_Why are there simple rules in a complicated universe.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {0016-3287},
Journal = {Futures},
Keywords = {chaos theory, complexity theory, complex systems, read-next, science},
Month = jul,
Number = {6},
Pages = {648--664},
Title = {Why are there simple rules in a complicated universe?},
Volume = {26},
Year = {1994},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0016-3287(94)90035-3}}
@inproceedings{schwartz_mapping_2013,
Abstract = {Imaging neuroscience links brain activation maps to behavior and cognition via correlational studies. Due to the nature of the individual experiments, based on eliciting neural response from a small number of stimuli, this link is incomplete, and unidirectional from the causal point of view. To come to conclusions on the function implied by the activation of brain regions, it is necessary to combine a wide exploration of the various brain functions and some inversion of the statistical inference. Here we introduce a methodology for accumulating knowledge towards a bidirectional link between observed brain activity and the corresponding function. We rely on a large corpus of imaging studies and a predictive engine. Technically, the challenges are to find commonality between the studies without denaturing the richness of the corpus. The key elements that we contribute are labeling the tasks performed with a cognitive ontology, and modeling the long tail of rare paradigms in the corpus. To our knowledge, our approach is the first demonstration of predicting the cognitive content of completely new brain images. To that end, we propose a method that predicts the experimental paradigms across different studies.},
Author = {Schwartz, Yannick and Thirion, Bertrand and Varoquaux, Ga{\"e}l},
File = {Schwartz et al_2013_Mapping cognitive ontologies to and from the brain.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/HRQZJN9C/Schwartz et al_2013_Mapping cognitive ontologies to and from the brain.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/ZMKZAUG6/en.html:text/html},
Keywords = {fMRI, forward inference, meta-analyses, multilabel learning, read-ok, reverse inference},
Month = nov,
Title = {Mapping cognitive ontologies to and from the brain},
Year = {2013}}
@article{mesoudi_bias_2006,
Abstract = {Evolutionary theories concerning the origins of human intelligence suggest that cultural transmission might be biased toward social over non-social information. This was tested by passing social and non-social information along multiple chains of participants. Experiment 1 found that gossip, defined as information about intense third-party social relationships, was transmitted with siginificantly greater accuracy and in significantly greater quantity than equivalent non-social information concerning individual behaviour or the physical environment. Experiment 2 replicated this finding controlling for narrative coherence, and additionally found that information concerning everyday non-gossip social interactions was transmitted just as well as the intense gossip interactions. It was therefore concluded that human cultural transmission is biased toward information concerning social interactions over equivalent non-social information.},
Author = {Mesoudi, Alex and Whiten, Andrew and Dunbar, Robin},
Copyright = {2006 The British Psychological Society},
Doi = {10.1348/000712605X85871},
File = {Mesoudi et al_2006_A bias for social information in human cultural transmission.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/N2W5WIN2/Mesoudi et al_2006_A bias for social information in human cultural transmission.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/6SX49A58/abstract.html:text/html},
Issn = {2044-8295},
Journal = {British Journal of Psychology},
Keywords = {Cultural evolution, cultural transmission, read-abstract, social cognition, social interaction},
Language = {en},
Month = aug,
Number = {3},
Pages = {405--423},
Title = {A bias for social information in human cultural transmission},
Volume = {97},
Year = {2006},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712605X85871}}
@inproceedings{mckinney_data_2010,
title = {Data {{Structures}} for {{Statistical Computing}} in {{Python}}},
volume = {445},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the 9th {{Python}} in {{Science Conference}}},
author = {McKinney, Wes},
year = {2010},
pages = {51--56},
file = {McKinney_2010_Data Structures for Statistical Computing in Python.pdf:/.mozilla/firefox/de5u065w.default/zotero/storage/T7TB5GC3/McKinney_2010_Data Structures for Statistical Computing in Python.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/.mozilla/firefox/de5u065w.default/zotero/storage/DRFW2QUG/mckinney.html:text/html}}
@misc{scott_barry_kaufman_mind_2013,
Author = {{Scott Barry Kaufman} and {Rebecca L. McMillan} and {Jerom L. Singer}},
File = {Mind Wandering\: A New Personal Intelligence Perspective | Beautiful Minds, Scientific American Blog Network:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/CGQSZH62/mind-wandering-a-new-personal-intelligence-perspective.html:text/html},
Keywords = {abstract reasoning, daydreaming, default mode network, episodic memories, goal directed behavior, goals, intelligence, IQ, mind wandering, positive constructive daydreaming},
Month = sep,
Title = {Mind {Wandering}: {A} {New} {Personal} {Intelligence} {Perspective} {\textbar} {Beautiful} {Minds}, {Scientific} {American} {Blog} {Network}},
Year = {2013}}
@article{mauss_les_1936,
Author = {Mauss, Marcel},
File = {Mauss_1936_Les techniques du corps.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/A8MGVU2B/Mauss_1936_Les techniques du corps.pdf:application/pdf},
Journal = {Journal de Psychologie},
Number = {3-4},
Title = {Les techniques du corps},
Volume = {32},
Year = {1936}}
@article{julien_bonhomme_alerte_2009,
Author = {{Julien Bonhomme}},
File = {Bonhomme, 2009, Alerte aux voleurs de sexe ! Anthropologie pragmatique d'une rumeur africaine.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/6ESV6U6I/Bonhomme, 2009, Alerte aux voleurs de sexe ! Anthropologie pragmatique d'une rumeur africaine.pdf:application/pdf},
Title = {Alerte aux voleurs de sexe ! {Anthropologie} pragmatique d'une rumeur africaine},
Year = {2009}}
@misc{weide_cmu_1998,
title = {The {{CMU Pronouncing Dictionary}}},
timestamp = {2016-06-30T16:06:23Z},
urldate = {2016-06-30},
url = {http://www.speech.cs.cmu.edu/cgi-bin/cmudict},
author = {Weide, Robert},
year = {1998},
keywords = {word features,word pronunciation},
file = {The CMU Pronouncing Dictionary:/.mozilla/firefox/de5u065w.default/zotero/storage/4FK9P32E/cmudict.html:text/html}}
@misc{thomas_a._schreiber_usf_2012,
Author = {{Thomas A. Schreiber}},
File = {USF Free Association Norms\: Introduction:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/PB82SIF9/Intro.html:text/html},
Keywords = {free association, network, word association},
Month = apr,
Title = {{USF} {Free} {Association} {Norms}: {Introduction}},
Year = {2012}}
@incollection{becker_epistemology_1996,
Abstract = {compare qualitative and ethnographic methods with those which are quantitative and survey / talk about how ethnographers have produced credible, believable results, especially those results which have continued to command respect and belief some thoughts about epistemology / why do we think there's a difference / many ethnographies / the actor's point of view: accuracy / the everyday world: making room for the unanticipated / full description, thick description: watching the margins},
Address = {Chicago, IL, US},
Author = {Becker, Howard S.},
Booktitle = {Ethnography and human development: {Context} and meaning in social inquiry},
Copyright = {(c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved},
Editor = {Jessor, R. and Colby, A. and Shweder, R. A.},
File = {APA PsycNET Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/HZN3WSJM/1996-98217-003.html:text/html;Becker_1996_The epistemology of qualitative research.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/JGP29NTC/Becker_1996_The epistemology of qualitative research.pdf:application/pdf},
Isbn = {978-0-226-39902-7 978-0-226-39903-4},
Keywords = {*Epistemology, *Ethnography, epistemological unification, Experimental Methods, qualitative research, quantitative research, read-ok},
Pages = {53--71},
Publisher = {University of Chicago Press},
Series = {The {John} {D}. and {Catherine} {T}. {MacArthur} {Foundation} series on mental health and development.},
Title = {The epistemology of qualitative research},
Year = {1996}}
@article{bangerter_transformation_2000,
Abstract = {The social representation (SR) of conception was investigated using an adapted version of Bartlett's (1932) method of serial reproduction. A sample of 75 participants reproduced a text describing the conception process in 20 segregated chains of four reproductive generations. Changes in sentence structure and content were analysed. Results indicated that when the scientific representation of conception is apprehended by laypersons, two different processes take place. First, the abstract biological description of the process is progressively transformed into an anthropomorphic description centred on the sperm and ovum (personification). Second, stereotypical sex-role attributes are projected onto the sperm and ovum. Limitations of the method of serial reproduction are discussed, as well as its potential for modelling processes of cultural diffusion of knowledge.},
Author = {Bangerter, Adrian},
Copyright = {2000 The British Psychological Society},
Doi = {10.1348/014466600164615},
File = {Bangerter_2000_Transformation between scientific and social representations of conception.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/W5XTBBDB/Bangerter_2000_Transformation between scientific and social representations of conception.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/SMXB2Z6Z/abstract.html:text/html},
Issn = {2044-8309},
Journal = {British Journal of Social Psychology},
Keywords = {Cultural evolution, read-ok, serial reproduction, social representation, transmission chains},
Language = {en},
Month = dec,
Number = {4},
Pages = {521--535},
Shorttitle = {Transformation between scientific and social representations of conception},
Title = {Transformation between scientific and social representations of conception: {The} method of serial reproduction},
Volume = {39},
Year = {2000},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466600164615}}
@article{landauer_latent_2008,
Author = {Landauer, Thomas and Dumais, Susan},
Doi = {10.4249/scholarpedia.4356},
Issn = {1941-6016},
Journal = {Scholarpedia},
Keywords = {natural language processing, read-abstract, Semantics},
Language = {en},
Number = {11},
Pages = {4356},
Title = {Latent semantic analysis},
Volume = {3},
Year = {2008},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.4249/scholarpedia.4356}}
@article{romney_culture_1996,
Abstract = {Culture consists of shared cognitive representations in the minds of individuals. This paper investigates the extent to which English speakers share the "same" semantic structure of English kinship terms. The semantic structure is defined as the arrangement of the terms relative to each other as represented in a metric space in which items judged more similar are placed closer to each other than items judged as less similar. The cognitive representation of the semantic structure, residing in the mind of an individual, is measured by judged similarity tasks involving comparisons among terms. Using six independent measurements, from each of 122 individuals, correspondence analysis represents the data in a common multidimensional spatial representation. Judged by a variety of statistical procedures, the individuals in our sample share virtually identical cognitive representations of the semantic structure of kinship terms. This model of culture accounts for 70-90\% of the total variability in these data. We argue that our findings on kinship should generalize to all semantic domains--e.g., animals, emotions, etc. The investigation of semantic domains is important because they may reside in localized functional units in the brain, because they relate to a variety of cognitive processes, and because they have the potential to provide methods for diagnosing individual breakdowns in the structure of cognitive representations typical of such ailments as Alzheimer disease.},
Author = {Romney, A. K. and Boyd, J. P. and Moore, C. C. and Batchelder, W. H. and Brazill, T. J.},
File = {Romney et al_1996_Culture as shared cognitive representations.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/URKP34BJ/Romney et al_1996_Culture as shared cognitive representations.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/W7RR5845/4699.html:text/html},
Issn = {0027-8424, 1091-6490},
Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences},
Keywords = {cognitive representation, culture, read-ok},
Language = {en},
Month = may,
Number = {10},
Pages = {4699--4705},
Pmid = {11607678},
Title = {Culture as shared cognitive representations},
Volume = {93},
Year = {1996}}
@misc{_systems_2015,
Abstract = {Systems theory is the interdisciplinary study of systems in general, with the goal of discovering patterns and elucidating principles that can be discerned from, and applied to, all types of systems at all nesting levels in all fields of research. Systems theory can reasonably be considered a specialization of systems thinking or as the goal output of systems science and systems engineering, with an emphasis on generality useful across a broad range of systems (versus the particular models of individual fields).
A central topic of systems theory is self-regulating systems, i.e. systems self-correcting through feedback. Self-regulating systems are found in nature, including the physiological systems of our body, in local and global ecosystems, and in climate---and in human learning processes (from the individual on up through international organizations like the UN).},
Copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/8REX22FD/index.html:text/html},
Journal = {Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia},
Keywords = {read-part},
Language = {en},
Month = sep,
Note = {Page Version ID: 679805822},
Title = {Systems theory},
Year = {2015}}
@book{west-eberhard_developmental_2003,
Address = {Oxford; New York},
Author = {West-Eberhard, Mary Jane},
Isbn = {978-0-19-512234-3 978-0-19-512235-0},
Keywords = {developmental biology, evo-devo, genetics},
Language = {English},
Publisher = {Oxford University Press},
Title = {Developmental plasticity and evolution},
Year = {2003}}
@article{prentice_pluralistic_1993,
Abstract = {Four studies examined the relation between college students' own attitudes toward alcohol use and their estimates of the attitudes of their peers. All studies found widespread evidence of pluralistic ignorance: Students believed that they were more uncomfortable with campus alcohol practices than was the average student. Study 2 demonstrated this perceived self--other difference also with respect to one's friends. Study 3 tracked attitudes toward drinking over the course of a semester and found gender differences in response to perceived deviance: Male students shifted their attitudes over time in the direction of what they mistakenly believed to be the norm, whereas female students showed no such attitude changes. Study 4 found that students' perceived deviance correlated with various measures of campus alienation, even though that deviance was illusory. The implications of these results for general issues of norm estimation and responses to perceived deviance are discussed.},
Author = {Prentice, Deborah A. and Miller, Dale T.},
Copyright = {(c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved},
Doi = {10.1037/0022-3514.64.2.243},
File = {Prentice_Miller_1993_Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/X32UQ9P3/Prentice_Miller_1993_Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {1939-1315(ELECTRONIC);0022-3514(PRINT)},
Journal = {Journal of Personality and Social Psychology},
Keywords = {Alcohol Drinking Attitudes, College Students, read-ok, Self Perception, Social Norms, Social Perception},
Number = {2},
Pages = {243--256},
Shorttitle = {Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus},
Title = {Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: {Some} consequences of misperceiving the social norm},
Volume = {64},
Year = {1993},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.2.243}}
@article{pinker_so_2005,
Abstract = {Abstract: In my book How the Mind Works, I defended the theory that the human mind is a naturally selected system of organs of computation. Jerry Fodor claims that `the mind doesn't work that way'(in a book with that title) because (1) Turing Machines cannot duplicate humans' ability to perform abduction (inference to the best explanation); (2) though a massively modular system could succeed at abduction, such a system is implausible on other grounds; and (3) evolution adds nothing to our understanding of the mind. In this review I show that these arguments are flawed. First, my claim that the mind is a computational system is different from the claim Fodor attacks (that the mind has the architecture of a Turing Machine); therefore the practical limitations of Turing Machines are irrelevant. Second, Fodor identifies abduction with the cumulative accomplishments of the scientific community over millennia. This is very different from the accomplishments of human common sense, so the supposed gap between human cognition and computational models may be illusory. Third, my claim about biological specialization, as seen in organ systems, is distinct from Fodor's own notion of encapsulated modules, so the limitations of the latter are irrelevant. Fourth, Fodor's arguments dismissing of the relevance of evolution to psychology are unsound.},
Author = {Pinker, Steven},
Doi = {10.1111/j.0268-1064.2005.00274.x},
File = {Pinker_2005_So How Does the Mind Work.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/29REVU9J/Pinker_2005_So How Does the Mind Work.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/XEGEMGSM/abstract.html:text/html},
Issn = {1468-0017},
Journal = {Mind \& Language},
Keywords = {evolutionary psychology, language, mind, pinker, read-next},
Language = {en},
Month = feb,
Number = {1},
Pages = {1--24},
Title = {So {How} {Does} the {Mind} {Work}?},
Volume = {20},
Year = {2005},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0268-1064.2005.00274.x}}
@misc{_semiotics_2015,
Abstract = {Semiotics (also called semiotic studies; not to be confused with the Saussurean tradition called semiology which is a part of semiotics) is the study of meaning-making, the study of sign processes and meaningful communication. This includes the study of signs and sign processes (semiosis), indication, designation, likeness, analogy, metaphor, symbolism, signification, and communication.
Semiotics is closely related to the field of linguistics, which, for its part, studies the structure and meaning of language more specifically. The semiotic tradition explores the study of signs and symbols as a significant part of communications. As different from linguistics, however, semiotics also studies non-linguistic sign systems. Semiotics is often divided into three branches:
Semantics: relation between signs and the things to which they refer; their signified denotata, or meaning
Syntactics: relations among or between signs in formal structures
Pragmatics: relation between signs and sign-using agents or interpreters
Semiotics is frequently seen as having important anthropological dimensions; for example, Umberto Eco proposes that every cultural phenomenon may be studied as communication. Some semioticians focus on the logical dimensions of the science, however. They examine areas belonging also to the life sciences---such as how organisms make predictions about, and adapt to, their semiotic niche in the world (see semiosis). In general, semiotic theories take signs or sign systems as their object of study: the communication of information in living organisms is covered in biosemiotics (including zoosemiotics).
Syntactics is the branch of semiotics that deals with the formal properties of signs and symbols. More precisely, syntactics deals with the "rules that govern how words are combined to form phrases and sentences".
Charles Morris adds that semantics deals with the relation of signs to their designata and the objects that they may or do denote; and, pragmatics deals with the biotic aspects of semiosis, that is, with all the psychological, biological, and sociological phenomena that occur in the functioning of signs.},
Copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/99Z759J9/index.html:text/html},
Journal = {Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia},
Keywords = {read-ok},
Language = {en},
Month = dec,
Note = {Page Version ID: 693547014},
Title = {Semiotics},
Year = {2015}}
@article{unsworth_automated_2005,
Author = {Unsworth, Nash and Heitz, Richard P. and Schrock, Josef C. and Engle, Randall W.},
Doi = {10.3758/BF03192720},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/N3CP6X7S/BF03192720.html:text/html;Unsworth et al_2005_An automated version of the operation span task.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/ZSDMGZ6X/Unsworth et al_2005_An automated version of the operation span task.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {1554-351X, 1554-3528},
Journal = {Behavior Research Methods},
Keywords = {Cognitive Psychology, memory span, openation span, read-abstract, reading span, word span, working memory},
Language = {en},
Month = aug,
Number = {3},
Pages = {498--505},
Title = {An automated version of the operation span task},
Volume = {37},
Year = {2005},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03192720}}
@article{unsworth_is_2015,
Abstract = {The relations between video-game experience and cognitive abilities were examined in the current study. In two experiments, subjects performed a number of working memory, fluid intelligence, and attention-control measures and filled out a questionnaire about their video-game experience. In Experiment 1, an extreme-groups analysis indicated that experienced video-game players outperformed nonplayers on several cognitive-ability measures. However, in Experiments 1 and 2, when analyses examined the full range of subjects at both the task level and the latent-construct level, nearly all of the relations between video-game experience and cognitive abilities were near zero. These results cast doubt on recent claims that playing video games leads to enhanced cognitive abilities. Statistical and methodological issues with prior studies of video-game experience are discussed along with recommendations for future studies.},
Author = {Unsworth, Nash and Redick, Thomas S. and McMillan, Brittany D. and Hambrick, David Z. and Kane, Michael J. and Engle, Randall W.},
Doi = {10.1177/0956797615570367},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/FPXJND5C/0956797615570367.html:text/html;Unsworth et al_2015_Is Playing Video Games Related to Cognitive Abilities.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/MRPQSHKR/Unsworth et al_2015_Is Playing Video Games Related to Cognitive Abilities.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {0956-7976, 1467-9280},
Journal = {Psychological Science},
Keywords = {cognitive ability, individual differences, memory span, read-abstract, reading span, word span, working memory},
Language = {en},
Month = apr,
Pages = {0956797615570367},
Pmid = {25896420},
Title = {Is {Playing} {Video} {Games} {Related} to {Cognitive} {Abilities}?},
Year = {2015},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797615570367}}
@article{omahony_emergence_2007,
Abstract = {Little is known about how communities producing collective goods govern themselves. In a multimethod study of one open source software community, we found that members developed a shared basis of formal authority but limited it with democratic mechanisms that enabled experimentation with shifting conceptions of authority over time. When members settled on a shared conception of authority, it was more expansive than their original design. A statistical test of the predictors of leadership reinforced this finding. By blending bureaucratic and democratic mechanisms, the governance system evolved with the community's changing conceptions of authority.},
Author = {O'Mahony, Siobh{\'a}n and Ferraro, Fabrizio},
Doi = {10.5465/AMJ.2007.27169153},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/64WNSU3A/1079.html:text/html;The-emergence-of-governance-in-an-open-source-community.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/9UKRDF3J/The-emergence-of-governance-in-an-open-source-community.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {0001-4273, 1948-0989},
Journal = {Academy of Management Journal},
Keywords = {COMMUNITY relations, DEMOCRACY, FREE computer software, INTELLECTUAL cooperation, LEADERSHIP, OPEN source software},
Language = {en},
Month = oct,
Number = {5},
Pages = {1079--1106},
Title = {The {Emergence} of {Governance} in an {Open} {Source} {Community}},
Volume = {50},
Year = {2007},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMJ.2007.27169153}}
@book{geertz_interpretation_1973,
Abstract = {In The Interpretation of Cultures, the most original anthropologist of his generation moved far beyond the traditional confines of his discipline to develop an important new concept of culture. This groundbreaking book, winner of the 1974 Sorokin Award of the American Sociological Association, helped define for an entire generation of anthropologists what their field is ultimately about.},
Author = {Geertz, Clifford},
File = {Geertz_1973_The Interpretation of Cultures.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/846TXHDU/Geertz_1973_The Interpretation of Cultures.pdf:application/pdf},
Isbn = {0-465-09719-7},
Keywords = {read-next, Social Science / Anthropology / Cultural, Social Science / Sociology / General},
Language = {en},
Publisher = {Basic Books},
Shorttitle = {The {Interpretation} of {Cultures}},
Title = {The {Interpretation} of {Cultures}: {Selected} {Essays}},
Year = {1973}}
@article{friston_action_2011,
Abstract = {We have suggested that the mirror-neuron system might be usefully understood as implementing Bayes-optimal perception of actions emitted by oneself or others. To substantiate this claim, we present neuronal simulations that show the same representations can prescribe motor behavior and encode motor intentions during action--observation. These simulations are based on the free-energy formulation of active inference, which is formally related to predictive coding. In this scheme, (generalised) states of the world are represented as trajectories. When these states include motor trajectories they implicitly entail intentions (future motor states). Optimizing the representation of these intentions enables predictive coding in a prospective sense. Crucially, the same generative models used to make predictions can be deployed to predict the actions of self or others by simply changing the bias or precision (i.e. attention) afforded to proprioceptive signals. We illustrate these points using simulations of handwriting to illustrate neuronally plausible generation and recognition of itinerant (wandering) motor trajectories. We then use the same simulations to produce synthetic electrophysiological responses to violations of intentional expectations. Our results affirm that a Bayes-optimal approach provides a principled framework, which accommodates current thinking about the mirror-neuron system. Furthermore, it endorses the general formulation of action as active inference.},
Author = {Friston, Karl and Mattout, Jeremie and Kilner, James},
Doi = {10.1007/s00422-011-0424-z},
File = {PubMed Central Full Text PDF:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/Q7NJXPU6/Friston et al. - 2011 - Action understanding and active inference.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {0340-1200},
Journal = {Biological cybernetics},
Keywords = {action, bayesian inference, cognitive science, inference},
Month = feb,
Number = {1-2},
Pages = {137--160},
Title = {Action understanding and active inference},
Volume = {104},
Year = {2011},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00422-011-0424-z}}
@article{perez_ipython:_2007,
Abstract = {Python offers basic facilities for interactive work and a comprehensive library on top of which more sophisticated systems can be built. The IPython project provides an enhanced interactive environment that includes, among other features, support for data visualization and facilities for distributed and parallel computation.},
Author = {P{\'e}rez, Fernando and Granger, Brian E.},
Doi = {10.1109/MCSE.2007.53},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/4W54ZUUV/MCSE.2007.html:text/html},
Issn = {1521-9615},
Journal = {Computing in Science \& Engineering},
Keywords = {Data visualization},
Month = may,
Number = {3},
Pages = {21--29},
Shorttitle = {{IPython}},
Title = {{IPython}: {A} {System} for {Interactive} {Scientific} {Computing}},
Volume = {9},
Year = {2007},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/MCSE.2007.53}}
@book{weber_success_2005,
Abstract = {Much of the innovative programming that powers the Internet, creates operating systems, and produces software is the result of "open source" code, that is, code that is freely distributed--as opposed to being kept secret--by those who write it. Leaving source code open has generated some of the most sophisticated developments in computer technology, including, most notably, Linux and Apache, which pose a significant challenge to Microsoft in the marketplace. As Steven Weber discusses, open source's success in a highly competitive industry has subverted many assumptions about how businesses are run, and how intellectual products are created and protected. Traditionally, intellectual property law has allowed companies to control knowledge and has guarded the rights of the innovator, at the expense of industry-wide cooperation. In turn, engineers of new software code are richly rewarded; but, as Weber shows, in spite of the conventional wisdom that innovation is driven by the promise of individual and corporate wealth, ensuring the free distribution of code among computer programmers can empower a more effective process for building intellectual products. In the case of Open Source, independent programmers--sometimes hundreds or thousands of them--make unpaid contributions to software that develops organically, through trial and error. Weber argues that the success of open source is not a freakish exception to economic principles. The open source community is guided by standards, rules, decisionmaking procedures, and sanctioning mechanisms. Weber explains the political and economic dynamics of this mysterious but important market development.},
Address = {Cambridge, MA},
Author = {Weber, Steven},
File = {The Success of Open Source - StevenWeberSuccess_of_Open_Source.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/W5CB26QA/StevenWeberSuccess_of_Open_Source.pdf:application/pdf},
Isbn = {978-0-674-01858-7},
Keywords = {community, free software, open source, read-ok, sociology},
Language = {English},
Month = nov,
Publisher = {Harvard University Press},
Title = {The {Success} of {Open} {Source}},
Year = {2005}}
@article{passonneau_measuring_2006,
Abstract = {Annotation projects dealing with complex semantic or pragmatic phenomena face the dilemma of creating annotation schemes that oversimplify the phenomena, or that capture distinctions conventional reliability metrics cannot measure adequately. The solution to the dilemma is to develop metrics that quantify the decisions that annotators are asked to make. This paper discusses MASI, distance metric for comparing sets, and illustrates its use in quantifying the reliability of a specific dataset. Annotations of Summary Content Units (SCUs) generate models referred to as pyramids which can be used to evaluate unseen human summaries or machine summaries. The paper presents reliability results for five pairs of pyramids created for document sets from the 2003 Document Understanding Conference (DUC). The annotators worked independently of each other. Differences between application of MASI to pyramid annotation and its previous application to co-reference annotation are discussed. In addition, it is argued that a paradigmatic reliability study should relate measures of inter-annotator agreement to independent assessments, such as significance tests of the annotated variables with respect to other phenomena. In effect, what counts as sufficiently reliable intera-annotator agreement depends on the use the annotated data will be put to.},
Author = {Passonneau, Rebecca},
File = {Passonneau_2006_Measuring Agreement on Set-valued Items (MASI) for Semantic and Pragmatic.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/2MVWA684/Passonneau_2006_Measuring Agreement on Set-valued Items (MASI) for Semantic and Pragmatic.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/JMJN3ZBW/ac162832.html:text/html},
Keywords = {linguistics, metric, read-abstract, set distance},
Title = {Measuring {Agreement} on {Set}-valued {Items} ({MASI}) for {Semantic} and {Pragmatic} {Annotation}},
Year = {2006}}
@article{griffiths_google_2007,
Abstract = {Human memory and Internet search engines face a shared computational problem, needing to retrieve stored pieces of information in response to a query. We explored whether they employ similar solutions, testing whether we could predict human performance on a fluency task using PageRank, a component of the Google search engine. In this task, people were shown a letter of the alphabet and asked to name the first word beginning with that letter that came to mind. We show that PageRank, computed on a semantic network constructed from word-association data, outperformed word frequency and the number of words for which a word is named as an associate as a predictor of the words that people produced in this task. We identify two simple process models that could support this apparent correspondence between human memory and Internet search, and relate our results to previous rational models of memory.},
Author = {Griffiths, Thomas L. and Steyvers, Mark and Firl, Alana},
Doi = {10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02027.x},
File = {Griffiths et al_2007_Google and the Mind Predicting Fluency With PageRank.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/Z6BS2H2S/Griffiths et al_2007_Google and the Mind Predicting Fluency With PageRank.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/256JVZ99/1069.html:text/html},
Issn = {0956-7976, 1467-9280},
Journal = {Psychological Science},
Keywords = {pagerank, read-ok, word association},
Language = {en},
Month = dec,
Number = {12},
Pages = {1069--1076},
Pmid = {18031414},
Title = {Google and the {Mind} {Predicting} {Fluency} {With} {PageRank}},
Volume = {18},
Year = {2007},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02027.x}}
@misc{_modularity_2014,
Abstract = {Modularity of mind is the notion that a mind may, at least in part, be composed of innate neural structures or modules which have distinct established evolutionarily developed functions. Somewhat different definitions of "module" have been proposed by different authorities.},
Copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/CPTBWGZG/index.html:text/html},
Journal = {Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia},
Keywords = {evolutionary psychology, fodor, mind, modularity, read-ok},
Language = {en},
Month = oct,
Title = {Modularity of mind},
Year = {2014}}
@article{hinton_reducing_2006,
Abstract = {High-dimensional data can be converted to low-dimensional codes by training a multilayer neural network with a small central layer to reconstruct high-dimensional input vectors. Gradient descent can be used for fine-tuning the weights in such ``autoencoder'' networks, but this works well only if the initial weights are close to a good solution. We describe an effective way of initializing the weights that allows deep autoencoder networks to learn low-dimensional codes that work much better than principal components analysis as a tool to reduce the dimensionality of data.},
Author = {Hinton, G. E. and Salakhutdinov, R. R.},
Doi = {10.1126/science.1127647},
File = {Full Text PDF:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/U3DKX2Z2/Hinton und Salakhutdinov - 2006 - Reducing the Dimensionality of Data with Neural Ne.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/JBNVH76Z/504.html:text/html},
Issn = {0036-8075, 1095-9203},
Journal = {Science},
Keywords = {deep learning, neural networks},
Language = {en},
Month = jul,
Number = {5786},
Pages = {504--507},
Title = {Reducing the {Dimensionality} of {Data} with {Neural} {Networks}},
Volume = {313},
Year = {2006},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1127647}}
@book{ingold_making_2013,
Author = {Ingold, Tim},
File = {Ingold_2013_Making.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/X4TEKIFE/Ingold_2013_Making.pdf:application/pdf},
Title = {Making},
Year = {2013}}
@incollection{gregg_word_1976,
Abstract = {Examines the effects of word frequency on recognition and recall and their implications for models of these memory processes. Two points are discussed in detail: (a) the extent to which it is possible to maintain the frequency paradox that recall is better for common than uncommon words, while in recognition the opposite is true; and (b) the support given to the retrieve-recognize model of recall by frequency effects. It is proposed that a multicomponent word model incorporating the encoding variability notion best fits the data.},
Address = {Oxford, England},
Author = {Gregg, Vernon},
Booktitle = {Recall and recognition},
Copyright = {(c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved},
File = {APA PsycNET Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/EA3PZB6T/1977-11959-007.html:text/html},
Isbn = {978-0-471-11229-7},
Keywords = {*Memory, *Recall (Learning), *Recognition (Learning), Word Frequency},
Publisher = {John Wiley \& Sons},
Title = {Word frequency, recognition and recall},
Year = {1976}}
@article{ducheneaut_socialization_2005,
Abstract = {Open Source Software (OSS) development is often characterized as a fundamentally new way to develop software. Past analyses and discussions, however, have treated OSS projects and their organization mostly as a static phenomenon. Consequently, we do not know how these communities of software developers are sustained and reproduced over time through the progressive integration of new members. To shed light on this issue I report on my analyses of socialization in a particular OSS community. In particular, I document the relationships OSS newcomers develop over time with both the social and material aspects of a project. To do so, I combine two mutually informing activities: ethnography and the use of software specially designed to visualize and explore the interacting networks of human and material resources incorporated in the email and code databases of OSS. Socialization in this community is analyzed from two perspectives: as an individual learning process and as a political process. From these analyses it appears that successful participants progressively construct identities as software craftsmen, and that this process is punctuated by specific rites of passage. Successful participants also understand the political nature of software development and progressively enroll a network of human and material allies to support their efforts. I conclude by discussing how these results could inform the design of software to support socialization in OSS projects, as well as practical implications for the future of these projects.},
Author = {Ducheneaut, Nicolas},
Doi = {10.1007/s10606-005-9000-1},
File = {Full Text PDF:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/VVA59E72/Ducheneaut - 2005 - Socialization in an Open Source Software Community.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/UKU6Q94G/10.html:text/html},
Issn = {0925-9724, 1573-7551},
Journal = {Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW)},
Keywords = {actor-network, Computer Science, general, Interdisciplinary Studies, learning, Open Source, read-ok, socialization, Social Sciences, general, software development},
Language = {en},
Month = aug,
Number = {4},
Pages = {323--368},
Shorttitle = {Socialization in an {Open} {Source} {Software} {Community}},
Title = {Socialization in an {Open} {Source} {Software} {Community}: {A} {Socio}-{Technical} {Analysis}},
Volume = {14},
Year = {2005},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10606-005-9000-1}}
@article{friston_predictive_2012,
Abstract = {Abstract Gotts, Chow and Martin provide a very nice review of repetition priming and suppression that reaches a compelling conclusion---we need to look more closely at synchronization in learning and priming. Indeed, current modeling work focuses on this issue---namely, the dynamic causal modeling of electrophysiological responses to address the role of synchrony in Bayesian explaining away. This commentary revisits the nature and relationships among the four theories in Gotts et al. and nuances some of their empirical predictions. In particular, I emphasize precision or uncertainty in predictive coding as a unifying consideration.},
Author = {Friston, Karl},
Doi = {10.1080/17588928.2012.691277},
File = {Full Text PDF:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/2J5HJQA3/Friston - 2012 - Predictive coding, precision and synchrony.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/7778JWEQ/17588928.2012.html:text/html},
Issn = {1758-8928},
Journal = {Cognitive Neuroscience},
Keywords = {bayesian brain, cognitive science, predictive coding},
Number = {3-4},
Pages = {238--239},
Title = {Predictive coding, precision and synchrony},
Volume = {3},
Year = {2012},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2012.691277}}
@misc{kieran_healy_social_2014,
Author = {{Kieran Healy}},
File = {Social Aspects of Success and Failure in Cultural Markets:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/QGEDVKV9/social-aspects-of-success-and-failure-in-cultural-markets.html:text/html},
Month = aug,
Title = {Social {Aspects} of {Success} and {Failure} in {Cultural} {Markets}},
Year = {2014}}
@article{schultz_resolving_2009,
Abstract = {ABSTRACT Anthropologists often disagree about whether, or in what ways, anthropology is ``evolutionary.'' Anthropologists defending accounts of primate or human biological development and evolution that conflict with mainstream ``neo-Darwinian'' thinking have sometimes been called ``creationists'' or have been accused of being ``antiscience.'' As a result, many cultural anthropologists struggle with an ``anti-antievolutionism'' dilemma: they are more comfortable opposing the critics of evolutionary biology, broadly conceived, than they are defending mainstream evolutionary views with which they disagree. Evolutionary theory, however, comes in many forms. Relational evolutionary approaches such as Developmental Systems Theory, niche construction, and autopoiesis--natural drift augment mainstream evolutionary thinking in ways that should prove attractive to many anthropologists who wish to affirm evolution but are dissatisfied with current ``neo-Darwinian'' hegemony. Relational evolutionary thinking moves evolutionary discussion away from reductionism and sterile nature--nurture debates and promises to enable fresh approaches to a range of problems across the subfields of anthropology. [Keywords: evolutionary anthropology, Developmental Systems Theory, niche construction, autopoeisis, natural drift]},
Author = {Schultz, Emily},
Doi = {10.1111/j.1548-1433.2009.01115.x},
File = {Schultz_2009_Resolving the Anti-Antievolutionism Dilemma.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/2X27H2TK/Schultz_2009_Resolving the Anti-Antievolutionism Dilemma.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/KFNGJDF9/abstract.html:text/html},
Issn = {1548-1433},
Journal = {American Anthropologist},
Keywords = {anthropology, developmental systems theory, evolution, read-abstract, read-next, relational world},
Language = {en},
Month = jun,
Number = {2},
Pages = {224--237},
Shorttitle = {Resolving the {Anti}-{Antievolutionism} {Dilemma}},
Title = {Resolving the {Anti}-{Antievolutionism} {Dilemma}: {A} {Brief} for {Relational} {Evolutionary} {Thinking} in {Anthropology}},
Volume = {111},
Year = {2009},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1433.2009.01115.x}}
@book{gould_ontogeny_1977,
Address = {Cambridge, Mass.},
Author = {Gould, Stephen Jay},
Isbn = {978-0-674-63940-9 978-0-674-63941-6},
Keywords = {developmental biology, evo-devo, evolution},
Language = {English},
Publisher = {Belknap Press of Harvard University Press},
Title = {Ontogeny and phylogeny},
Year = {1977}}
@article{morin_how_2013,
Abstract = {It has often been suggested that innate features of the human mind could make some cultural forms more successful than others. This paper presents a case study consistent with this ``cognitive attraction'' hypothesis. Numerous studies show that direct eye-gaze catches the attention of adults and newborns. Adults find it more attractive. We explore one possible cultural consequence of this cognitive appeal. Among XVIth century European portraits, direct-gaze paintings are more likely to be featured in today's art books. In Renaissance Europe, the proportion of paintings that stare at the viewer grows gradually, strongly, and remains prevalent for centuries. A demographic analysis of this shift shows that it was due to the arrival of new generations of painters. Those artists show a preference for direct-gaze portraits as soon as they start painting, suggesting that they acquired the new style in the years of their apprenticeship. The preferences of those painters and of contemporary art critics seem consistent with the innate attentional bias that favours direct-gaze faces. The structure of the ``Renaissance gaze shift'' bears evidence for the importance of demographic turn-over in cultural change.},
Author = {Morin, Olivier},
Doi = {10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.01.004},
File = {Morin_2013_How portraits turned their eyes upon us.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/T62WS2R9/Morin_2013_How portraits turned their eyes upon us.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/VA35KFJJ/abstract.html:text/html},
Issn = {1090-5138, 1879-0607},
Journal = {Evolution and Human Behavior},
Keywords = {Art history, Cognitive attraction, Cultural selection, Direct eye-gaze, Renaissance},
Language = {English},
Month = may,
Number = {3},
Pages = {222--229},
Shorttitle = {How portraits turned their eyes upon us},
Title = {How portraits turned their eyes upon us: {Visual} preferences and demographic change in cultural evolution},
Volume = {34},
Year = {2013},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.01.004}}
@book{damasio_descartes_2008,
Abstract = {In the centuries since Descartes famously proclaimed, 'I think, therefore I am,' science has often overlooked emotions as the source of a person's true being. Even modern neuroscience has tended until recently to concentrate on the cognitive aspects of brain function, disregarding emotions. This attitude began to change with the publication of Descartes' Error. Antonio Damasio challenged traditional ideas about the connection between emotions and rationality. In this wonderfully engaging book, Damasio takes the reader on a journey of scientific discovery through a series of case studies, demonstrating what many of us have long suspected: emotions are not a luxury, they are essential to rational thinking and to normal social behaviour.},
Author = {Damasio, Antonio},
File = {Damasio_2008_Descartes' Error.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/PPEIN9TD/Damasio_2008_Descartes' Error.pdf:application/pdf},
Isbn = {978-1-4070-7206-7},
Keywords = {reason, Science / General},
Language = {en},
Month = sep,
Publisher = {Random House},
Shorttitle = {Descartes' {Error}},
Title = {Descartes' {Error}: {Emotion}, {Reason} and the {Human} {Brain}},
Year = {2008}}
@misc{_shirky:_????,
File = {Shirky\: Power Laws, Weblogs, and Inequality:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/WK8WIX4V/powerlaw_weblog.html:text/html},
Keywords = {influence, power law, weblogs},
Title = {Shirky: {Power} {Laws}, {Weblogs}, and {Inequality}}}
@article{di_paolo_sensitivity_2008,
Abstract = {We introduce a series of evolutionary robotics simulations that address the behaviour of individuals in socially contingent interactions. The models are based on a recent study by Auvray, Lenay and Stewart [(2006). The attribution of intentionality in a simulated environment: The case of minimalist devices. In Tenth meeting of the association for the scientific study of consciousness, Oxford, UK, 23--26 June, 2006] on tactile perceptual crossing in a minimal virtual environment. In accordance, both the empirical experiments and our simulations point out the essential character of global embodied interaction dynamics for the sensitivity to contingency to arise. Rather than being individually perceived by any of the interactors, sensitivity to contingency arises from processes of circular causality that characterise the collective dynamics. Such global dynamical aspects are frequently neglected when studying social cognition. Furthermore, our synthetic studies point out interesting aspects of the task that are not immediately obvious in the empirical data. They, in addition, generate new hypotheses for further experiments. We conclude by promoting a minimal but tractable, dynamic and embodied account to social interaction, combining synthetic and empirical findings as well as concrete predictions regarding sensorimotor strategies, the role of time-delays and robustness to perturbations in interactive dynamics.},
Author = {Di Paolo, Ezequiel A. and Rohde, Marieke and Iizuka, Hiroyuki},
Doi = {10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.006},
File = {Di Paolo et al_2008_Sensitivity to social contingency or stability of interaction.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/9ZFIKZFU/Di Paolo et al_2008_Sensitivity to social contingency or stability of interaction.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/RJ25N395/S0732118X07000438.html:text/html},
Issn = {0732-118X},
Journal = {New Ideas in Psychology},
Keywords = {Agency, Evolutionary robotics, Interaction dynamics, Perceptual crossing, Simulation modelling, Social contingency},
Month = aug,
Number = {2},
Pages = {278--294},
Series = {Dynamics and {Psychology}},
Shorttitle = {Sensitivity to social contingency or stability of interaction?},
Title = {Sensitivity to social contingency or stability of interaction? {Modelling} the dynamics of perceptual crossing},
Volume = {26},
Year = {2008},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.006}}
@article{liben-nowell_tracing_2008,
Abstract = {Although information, news, and opinions continuously circulate in the worldwide social network, the actual mechanics of how any single piece of information spreads on a global scale have been a mystery. Here, we trace such information-spreading processes at a person-by-person level using methods to reconstruct the propagation of massively circulated Internet chain letters. We find that rather than fanning out widely, reaching many people in very few steps according to ``small-world'' principles, the progress of these chain letters proceeds in a narrow but very deep tree-like pattern, continuing for several hundred steps. This suggests a new and more complex picture for the spread of information through a social network. We describe a probabilistic model based on network clustering and asynchronous response times that produces trees with this characteristic structure on social-network data.},
Author = {Liben-Nowell, David and Kleinberg, Jon},
Doi = {10.1073/pnas.0708471105},
File = {Liben-Nowell_Kleinberg_2008_Tracing information flow on a global scale using Internet chain-letter data.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/M25B7JG8/Liben-Nowell_Kleinberg_2008_Tracing information flow on a global scale using Internet chain-letter data.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/62WPVJDG/4633.html:text/html},
Issn = {0027-8424, 1091-6490},
Journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences},
Keywords = {algorithms, diffusion in networks, epidemics, social networks},
Language = {en},
Month = mar,
Number = {12},
Pages = {4633--4638},
Pmid = {18353985},
Title = {Tracing information flow on a global scale using {Internet} chain-letter data},
Volume = {105},
Year = {2008},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0708471105}}
@article{tulving_subjective_1966,
title = {Subjective Organization and Effects of Repetition in Multi-Trial Free-Recall Learning},
volume = {5},
issn = {00225371},
doi = {10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80016-6},
language = {en},
timestamp = {2017-02-03T22:56:39Z},
number = {2},
urldate = {2017-02-03},
journal = {Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior},
author = {Tulving, Endel},
month = apr,
year = {1966},
pages = {193--197},
file = {Tulving_1966_Subjective organization and effects of repetition in multi-trial free-recall.pdf:/.mozilla/firefox/de5u065w.default/zotero/storage/49Q443HM/Tulving_1966_Subjective organization and effects of repetition in multi-trial free-recall.pdf:application/pdf}}
@article{tulving_functional_1968,
Author = {Tulving, Endel and Patterson, Roy D.},
File = {Tulving_Patterson_1968_Functional Units and Retrieval Processes in Free Recall.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/79UZKIBQ/Tulving_Patterson_1968_Functional Units and Retrieval Processes in Free Recall.pdf:application/pdf},
Keywords = {free recall, functional units, memory units, primary memory, read-abstract, retrieval processes, secondary memory},
Title = {Functional {Units} and {Retrieval} {Processes} in {Free} {Recall}},
Year = {1968}}
@article{soergel_rampant_2015,
Author = {Soergel, David A. W.},
Doi = {10.12688/f1000research.5930.2},
File = {Rampant software errors may undermine scientific results - F1000Research:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/3FMJNTKI/3-303.html:text/html;Soergel_2015_Rampant software errors may undermine scientific results.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/N9EIQD8X/Soergel_2015_Rampant software errors may undermine scientific results.pdf:application/pdf},
Issn = {2046-1402},
Journal = {F1000Research},
Keywords = {read-ok, software development, software errors},
Language = {en},
Month = jul,
Title = {Rampant software errors may undermine scientific results},
Year = {2015},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.5930.2}}
@article{santorini_part--speech_1990,
Author = {Santorini, Beatrice},
File = {"Part-of-Speech Tagging Guidelines for the Penn Treebank Project (3rd R" by Beatrice Santorini:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/BQUXHJK8/570.html:text/html;Santorini_1990_Part-of-Speech Tagging Guidelines for the Penn Treebank Project (3rd Revision).pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/ZPQ4SS73/Santorini_1990_Part-of-Speech Tagging Guidelines for the Penn Treebank Project (3rd Revision).pdf:application/pdf},
Journal = {Technical Reports (CIS)},
Month = jul,
Title = {Part-of-{Speech} {Tagging} {Guidelines} for the {Penn} {Treebank} {Project} (3rd {Revision})},
Year = {1990}}
@article{keuleers_megastudies_2015,
Abstract = {This paper introduces and summarizes the special issue on megastudies, crowdsourcing, and large datasets in psycholinguistics. We provide a brief historical overview and show how the papers in this issue have extended the field by compiling new databases and making important theoretical contributions. In addition, we discuss several studies that use text corpora to build distributional semantic models to tackle several interesting problems in psycholinguistics. Finally, as is the case across the papers, we highlight some methodological issues that are brought forth via the analyses of such datasets.},
Author = {Keuleers, Emmanuel and Balota, David A.},
Doi = {10.1080/17470218.2015.1051065},
Issn = {1747-0226},
Journal = {Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006)},
Language = {ENG},
Month = may,
Pages = {1--24},
Pmid = {25975773},
Shorttitle = {Megastudies, {Crowdsourcing}, and {Large} {Datasets} in {Psycholinguistics}},
Title = {Megastudies, {Crowdsourcing}, and {Large} {Datasets} in {Psycholinguistics}: {An} {Overview} {Of} {Recent} {Developments}},
Year = {2015},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1051065}}
@article{auvray_perceptual_2009,
Abstract = {How in real-life or through the use of technical devices can we recognize the presence of other persons and under what conditions can we differentiate them from objects? In order to approach this question, in the study reported here we explored the most basic conditions necessary for participants to recognize the presence of another person during a perceptual interaction. We created a mini-network of two minimalist devices and investigated whether participants were able to differentiate the perception of another person from the perception of a fixed and a mobile object even when the pattern of sensory stimulation was reduced to a bare minimum. We show that participants can recognize when the all-or-none tactile stimulation they experienced was attributable to an encounter with the other participant's avatar or the mobile object rather than with a fixed object. Participants were also able to establish different strategies in order to favor the situations of mutual perception. Thus, in the minimalist conditions of our experiment, the perception of another intentional subject was not based purely on any particular shape or objective trajectories of displacement; it was also based on properties that are intrinsic to the joint perceptual activity itself.},
Author = {Auvray, Malika and Lenay, Charles and Stewart, John},
Doi = {10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.12.002},
File = {Auvray et al_2009_Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/6Z49QCCR/Auvray et al_2009_Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment.pdf:application/pdf;ScienceDirect Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/QF9XW4KN/S0732118X07000748.html:text/html},
Issn = {0732-118X},
Journal = {New Ideas in Psychology},
Keywords = {Enaction, Intentionality, Interaction, Intersubjectivity, Participatory sense-making, Perceptual crossing, read-ok, Sensory substitution, Tactile perception},
Month = apr,
Number = {1},
Pages = {32--47},
Title = {Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment},
Volume = {27},
Year = {2009},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.12.002}}
@misc{_constructivist_2015,
Abstract = {Constructivist epistemology is a branch in philosophy of science maintaining that natural science consists of mental constructs that are constructed with the aim of explaining sensory experience (or measurements) of the natural world. According to it, scientific knowledge is constructed by the scientific community, seeking to measure and construct models of the natural world.},
Copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/KD88B4ZE/index.html:text/html},
Journal = {Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia},
Keywords = {edgar morin, read-part},
Language = {en},
Month = aug,
Note = {Page Version ID: 675967547},
Title = {Constructivist epistemology},
Year = {2015}}
@techreport{ohm_broken_2009,
Abstract = {Computer scientists have recently undermined our faith in the privacy-protecting power of anonymization, the name for techniques for protecting the privacy of individuals in large databases by deleting information like names and social security numbers. These scientists have demonstrated they can often 'reidentify' or 'deanonymize' individuals hidden in anonymized data with astonishing ease. By understanding this research, we will realize we have made a mistake, labored beneath a fundamental misunderstanding, which has assured us much less privacy than we have assumed. This mistake pervades nearly every information privacy law, regulation, and debate, yet regulators and legal scholars have paid it scant attention. We must respond to the surprising failure of anonymization, and this Article provides the tools to do so.},
Address = {Rochester, NY},
Author = {Ohm, Paul},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/U27JHUT9/papers.html:text/html;SSRN-id1450006.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/6AT9NSMR/SSRN-id1450006.pdf:application/pdf},
Institution = {Social Science Research Network},
Keywords = {anonymization, Data Protection Directive, deidentification, HIPAA, information privacy, privacy, reidentification},
Month = aug,
Number = {ID 1450006},
Shorttitle = {Broken {Promises} of {Privacy}},
Title = {Broken {Promises} of {Privacy}: {Responding} to the {Surprising} {Failure} of {Anonymization}},
Type = {{SSRN} {Scholarly} {Paper}},
Year = {2009}}
@article{hunter_matplotlib:_2007,
Abstract = {Scitation is the online home of leading journals and conference proceedings from AIP Publishing and AIP Member Societies},
Author = {Hunter, John D.},
Doi = {10.1109/MCSE.2007.55},
File = {Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/MK2CMSI3/MCSE.2007.html:text/html},
Issn = {1521-9615},
Journal = {Computing in Science \& Engineering},
Month = may,
Number = {3},
Pages = {90--95},
Shorttitle = {Matplotlib},
Title = {Matplotlib: {A} 2D {Graphics} {Environment}},
Volume = {9},
Year = {2007},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/MCSE.2007.55}}
@article{jaegher_participatory_2007,
Abstract = {As yet, there is no enactive account of social cognition. This paper extends the enactive concept of sense-making into the social domain. It takes as its departure point the process of interaction between individuals in a social encounter. It is a well-established finding that individuals can and generally do coordinate their movements and utterances in such situations. We argue that the interaction process can take on a form of autonomy. This allows us to reframe the problem of social cognition as that of how meaning is generated and transformed in the interplay between the unfolding interaction process and the individuals engaged in it. The notion of sense-making in this realm becomes participatory sense-making. The onus of social understanding thus moves away from strictly the individual only.},
Author = {Jaegher, Hanne De and Paolo, Ezequiel Di},
Doi = {10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9},
File = {Jaegher_Paolo_2007_Participatory sense-making.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/I7DNTK5S/Jaegher_Paolo_2007_Participatory sense-making.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/CJGAKBJ7/10.html:text/html},
Issn = {1568-7759, 1572-8676},
Journal = {Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences},
Keywords = {Artificial Intelligence (incl. Robotics), Autonomy, Coordination, Enaction, Interaction process, Interdisciplinary Studies, making sense, Participatory sense-making, Phenomenology, Philosophy of Mind, read-now, Sense-making, Social cognition},
Language = {en},
Month = dec,
Number = {4},
Pages = {485--507},
Title = {Participatory sense-making},
Volume = {6},
Year = {2007},
Bdsk-Url-1 = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9}}
@article{claidiere_role_2007,
Abstract = {Henrich and Boyd (2002) were the first to propose a formal model of the role of attraction in cultural evolution. They came to the surprising conclusion that, when both attraction and selection are at work, final outcomes are determined by selection alone. This result is based on a determistic view of cultural attraction, different from the probabilistic view introduced in Sperber (1996). We defend this probabilistic view, show how to model it, and argue that, when both attraction and selection are at work, both affect final outcomes.},
Author = {Claidi{\`e}re, Nicolas and Sperber, Dan},
Doi = {10.1163/156853707X171829},
File = {Claidi{\`e}re_Sperber_2007_The role of attraction in cultural evolution.pdf:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/H9EZ6FKW/Claidi{\`e}re_Sperber_2007_The role of attraction in cultural evolution.pdf:application/pdf;Snapshot:/home/sl/.mozilla/firefox/dv0e3tkv.default-1432918138968/zotero/storage/WRDS7769/156853707x171829.html:text/html},
Issn = {1568-5373},
Journal = {Journal of Cognition and Culture},
Keywords = {ATTRACTION, Cultural evolution, CULTURAL TRANSMISSION, SELECTION},
Month = mar,